Skip to content

This website will be sold

Contact: author@sexandsexology.com

Attitudes towards feminism predict susceptibility to feminism‐related fake news

2.1 Method

2.1.1 Participants

We recruited for this study via student email lists and social media posts and targeted as many responses as possible before an agreed stopping date. The study was completed by 1537 participants with a mean age of 26.05 years (SD = 9.22). There were no exclusion criteria, beyond being over 18 years old. Participants were mostly (60%) women (n = 920), with 565 men, 25 who reported their gender as other, and 27 who preferred not to say. Most participants (86%) were Irish nationals, and the remaining participants were from another EU country (9%) or outside of the EU (5%). When asked if they identified as a feminist, 58% agreed or strongly agreed, 21% disagreed or strongly disagreed, and 21% neither agreed nor disagreed. 87% of the sample said they used social media a few times a day or more, over the past year. Participants reported interest in the #MeToo movement, with 75% saying they were somewhat likely, likely or very likely to read an online article about the #MeToo movement if they came across it.

2.1.2 Materials

Feminism Attitudes. A scale to measure support for the feminist movement was developed for this study. Participants were asked to rate their agreement with five statements on a scale of (1) Strongly Agree to (5) Strongly Disagree; I identify as a feminist, I believe the feminist movement has gone too far (reverse scored), I support the feminist movement, I believe the feminist movement is necessary, I support the #MeToo movement. These were summed to create a Feminism Attitudes scale. Cronbach’s alpha indicated high reliability (α = .91). We purposefully did not define feminism for participants before they undertook the study, as we were interested in their existing perceptions of feminism (as they perceived it) and did not wish to influence responses.

News Stories. Participants were presented with 8 news stories in a random order. All stories were presented as an image followed by 2–3 lines of text.

True Stories. All participants saw the same six true stories; Bill Cosby convicted of sexual assault, Controversial comments about rape victims by Irish radio host George Hook, “House of Cards” cancelled after Kevin Spacey allegations emerge, Controversial comments made by actor Liam Neeson about the #MeToo movement, Google employee James Damore fired over an internal memo about discrimination, Donald Trump comments about a war on men in America. All true stories were presented with a general image of the subject of the story (e.g., a photograph of Liam Neeson giving an interview).

Fake Stories. All participants saw two fake stories; one concerning estimates of fabricated rape claims and one concerning a riot that broke out at a protest. As can be seen in Table 1, two versions of each story were created – one that aligned with feminist views (e.g., that there are low levels of fabricated rape claims, that men’s rights protestors were in the wrong) and one that did not align with feminist views (e.g., that there are higher levels of fabricated rape claims, that feminist protestors were in the wrong). All participants saw the feminism-aligned version of one story and the feminism-misaligned version of the other story. Both stories were entirely false. There have never been any studies to estimate rates of fabricated rape claims in Ireland. There was an “I Believe Her” protest in Dublin, as described in the riot story, but there was no violence, no injuries and no damage to property. Both versions of the riot story were accompanied by the same image of a man and woman shouting at each other at a protest and both versions of the rape statistics story were accompanied by the same image of an Irish courthouse.

TABLE 1.
The fake stories used in Experiment 1
Rape statistics story Riot story
Feminism-Aligned In October 2018, an article in the Irish Independent caused controversy by estimating that an average of just 4% of rape cases brought to the Irish courts are false. Following the acquittal of charges against two Ulster Rugby Players in the Belfast rape trial in March 2018, a large-scale “I Believe Her” protest on O’Connell Street in Dublin turned violent, with Men’s Rights counter-protesters assaulting a female protester, leading to a larger altercation causing extensive damage to local businesses and injuring two Gardaí.
Feminism-Misaligned In October 2018, an article in the Irish Independent caused controversy by estimating that an average of 16% of rape cases brought to the Irish courts are false. Following the acquittal of charges against two Ulster Rugby Players in the Belfast rape trial in March 2018, a large-scale “I Believe Her” protest on O’Connell Street in Dublin turned violent, with protestors assaulting a passer-by, setting off a riot which caused extensive damage to local businesses and injured two Gardaí.
  • Note: All participants saw one version of the rape statistics story and the other version of the riot story. Note that “Gardaí” (mentioned in the riot stories) is the term for Irish police officers.

For each story, participants were asked if they remembered the event and could choose from I remember seeing/hearing about this, I do not remember seeing/hearing this but I remember it happening, I do not remember this event but I believe it happened, I remember this event differently, I do not remember this event. As in previous work (Murphy et al., 2019), participants were classed as remembering an event if they indicated that they specifically remembered seeing it or they generally remembered it happening. They were classed as not remembering an event if they said they remembered the event differently or not at all. Unless otherwise stated, participants who stated that they merely believed an event had happened were excluded from the analysis (as in Murphy et al., 2019). This is to allow an assessment of false memories for an event, separate from belief in that event, given that these are distinct processes which do not always mutually occur (Otgaar et al., 2014; Wade et al., 2018).

After each story, participants were asked “How ultimately good/bad do you believe this event was for the feminist movement?” and could answer on a slider from 0 (very bad) to 100 (very good).

After viewing all eight stories, participants were told “Some people who completed this survey were shown fabricated news stories (news stories that didn’t happen, they were completely fabricated by the researchers). If you think any of the stories you saw were fake, please select them below” and were shown all stories again.

2.1.3 Procedure

The study was conducted entirely online, using the Qualtrics survey platform. Participants first completed the demographics and feminism attitudes questions. Then each participant viewed eight news stories (six true, two fake) presented in random order. Participants were then asked to pick out any stories that they believed were fabricated before they were debriefed. We used a debriefing procedure shown to be effective in a similar fake news study (Murphy et al., 2020). The study received ethical approval from the School of Applied Psychology Ethics Committee, University College Cork.

2.2 Results

Over half the sample (53%) falsely claimed to remember at least one fabricated event; 39% remembered one false event and a further 14% remembered two false events. Both versions of the riot story were remembered at a high rate (Feminist Riot = 51%; Men’s Rights Riot = 55%), with lower rates for both versions of the false rape claim statistics story (Low Rates = 29%; High Rates = 26%). This compares to a rate of reported memories ranging between 34% and 92% for the true stories (average = 3.6 true stories recalled, SD = 1.49).

When asked how good each fabricated event was for the feminist movement, the feminist riot was rated as significantly worse (M = 36.03, SD = 27.60) than the men’s rights riot (M = 52.44, SD = 28.91), t(1318), = 10.55, p < .001, d = 0.58. The article describing high rates of false rape claims was also rated as significantly worse for the feminist movement (M = 34.39, SD = 27.06) than the article describing lower rates (M = 45.85, SD = 28.80), t(1288), = 11.46, p < .001, d = 0.41. There was a very small, non-significant correlation between feminism attitudes and ratings for feminism-aligned stories (r[1300] = −.08, p = .006), with those who reported more negative attitudes towards feminism rating the stories as less positive. There was no such correlation for the feminism-misaligned events (r(1273) = −.004, p = .887). We classified participants as seeing a story as positive for feminists if it was 51 or above on the 1–100 scale and negative if it was 50 or below.1 Collapsing both feminism-aligned stories, 55% rated the story as expected (i.e., rated either the men’s rights riot or the low rates of false rape claims as positive). Collapsing both feminism-misaligned stories, 78% rated the story as expected (i.e., rated the feminist riot or the higher rates of false rape claims as negative). We split the file to separately analyse those who interpreted the events as expected (the feminism-aligned events positively/ the feminism-misaligned events negatively), vs. those who did not.

2.2.1 Do attitudes towards feminism predict reported memories for fabricated feminism-related events?

Responses to the two feminist-aligned stories were collapsed and binary logistic regressions were conducted to assess the effect of feminist attitudes on false memories. Analyses were conducted separately for those who interpreted the feminism-aligned story as positive for the feminist movement (n = 438) and those who viewed the story as negative (n = 594). A further 184 participants declined to rate the story and so are not included in either analysis. Those who reported a mere belief in the fabricated event are also excluded here.

For those who interpreted the story as expected, the model was statistically significant, χ2(1, N = 438) = 16.49, p < .001, R2 (Cox & Snell) = .04, R2 (Nagelkerke) = .05, and correctly classified 59% of cases. As shown in Table 2, there was a significant effect of feminism attitudes, such that for every one-point increase (i.e., more negative attitudes), the odds of reporting a memory for the feminism-aligned story were 8% lower. For those who did not interpret the story as expected, the model was not statistically significant, χ2(1, N = 594) = 1.60, p = .206, R2 (Cox & Snell) = .00, R2 (Nagelkerke) = .00. As shown in Table 2, there was no significant effect of feminism attitudes on memories for this group.

TABLE 2.
Results of four binary logistic regressions for false memories of the feminism-aligned events and separately, for the feminism-misaligned events from Experiment 1
Predictors b SE b Wald df p Exp(b) 95% C.I. (b)
Feminism-Aligned Stories Interpreted event as positive for feminism (n = 438) Feminism Attitudes −0.08 0.02 15.68 1 <.001 0.92 [0.89, 0.96]
Constant 0.95 0.25 14.29 1 <.001 2.59
Interpreted event as negative for feminism (n = 594) Feminism Attitudes 0.02 0.02 1.60 1 .206 1.02 [0.99, 1.06]
Constant −0.64 0.22 8.35 1 .004 0.52
Feminism-Misaligned Stories Interpreted event as negative for feminism (n = 817) Feminism Attitudes 0.05 0.02 11.78 1 .001 1.05 [1.02, 1.08]
Constant −1.10 0.19 33.43 1 <.001 0.33
Interpreted event as positive for feminism (n = 233) Feminism Attitudes 0.00 0.03 0.02 1 .886 1.00 [0.95, 1.06]
Constant 0.14 0.35 0.15 1 .698 1.14
  • Note: The models for those who interpreted the stories as expected are shown separately to those who did not interpret the stories as expected. Significant predictors are shown in bold.

Identical analyses were conducted on the rate of falsely reported memories for the feminism-misaligned stories. Analyses were again conducted separately for those who interpreted the feminism-misaligned story as negative for the feminist movement (n = 817) and those who viewed the story as positive (n = 233). A further 199 participants declined to rate the story and so are not included in either analysis. Those who reported a mere belief in the fabricated event were also excluded.

For those who interpreted the story as expected, the model was statistically significant, χ2(1, N = 817) = 11.88, p = .001, R2 (Cox & Snell) = .01, R2 (Nagelkerke) = .02, and correctly classified 62% of cases. As shown in Table 3, there was a significant effect of feminism attitudes, such that for every one-point increase (i.e., more negative attitudes), the odds of falsely reporting a memory for the feminism-misaligned story were 5% greater. For those who did not interpret the story as expected, the model was not statistically significant, χ2(1, N = 233) = 0.02, p = .886, R2 (Cox & Snell) = .00, R2 (Nagelkerke) = .00. As shown in Table 2, there was no significant effect of feminism attitudes on memories for this group.

Note that participants who reported a memory of hearing about the event are classed as “remembering” in the analyses shown in Table 2 (with mere beliefs excluded). If we used a more liberal classification of memory and included those who reported merely believing the event had occurred, the false memory rate would increase to 70% overall, with 35% reporting one false memory or belief and 35% reporting two false memories or beliefs. The regression results do not change when those who reported a memory or belief are included. For those who interpreted the story as expected, more negative feminist attitudes predicted significantly greater likelihood of a false memory or belief for the feminism-misaligned story (OR = 1.04, [95% CI: 1.01–1.07]) and reduced likelihood of a false memory or belief for the feminism-aligned story (OR = 0.91, [95% CI: 0.88–0.94]). This was not the case for those who did not interpret the event as expected.

For illustrative purposes, we grouped participants according to their score on the feminism attitudes scale (minimum 5, maximum 25); Support Feminism (5–12, n = 917), Neutral (13–17, n = 379), Oppose Feminism (18–25, n = 216). Figure 1 shows rates of reported memories for each fabricated event across these groups. Note that only those who interpreted the event as expected (e.g., positive events as positive for feminism) are included here, those who did not interpret the event as expected were excluded.

Reported memories for each of the four fabricated events (left) and correct identification of each of the four fabricated events as fake (right) in Experiment 1. Participants are grouped according to their scores on the Feminist Attitudes scale. The feminism-misaligned stories are shown in a dotted pattern

2.2.2 Do attitudes towards feminism predict ability to identify fabricated feminism-related events?

After responding to all the news stories, participants were told they may have been exposed to fake news and were asked to select any stories they thought were fabricated. The fabricated stories were selected as fake by the majority of participants; feminist riot 47%, men’s rights riot 47%, high false rape claims 52%, low false rape claims 58%. The true stories were selected as fake at a lower rate overall; George Hook 16%, Bill Cosby 4%, Donald Trump 12%, Google 43%, Liam Neeson 22%, Kevin Spacey 7%. Two binary logistic regressions were conducted to assess the impact of feminism attitudes on ability to identify the story as fake. As before, these were conducted separately for those who interpreted the story as expected and those who did not.

For the feminism-aligned events, the model for those who interpreted the story as expected was statistically significant, χ2(1, N = 577) = 28.22, p = <.001, R2 (Cox & Snell) = .05, R2 (Nagelkerke) = .06, and correctly classified 61% of cases. As shown in Table 3, there was a significant effect of feminism attitudes, such that for every one-point increase (i.e., more negative attitudes), the odds of participants identifying the feminism-aligned story as fake were 9% greater. For those who did not interpret the story as expected, the model was not statistically significant, χ2(1, N = 724) = 0.07, p = .787, R2 (Cox & Snell) = .00, R2 (Nagelkerke) = .00.

TABLE 3.
Results of four binary logistic regressions for ability to correctly identify the feminism-aligned and feminism misaligned events as fabricated in Experiment 1
Predictors b SE b Wald df p Exp(b) 95% C.I. (b)
Feminism-Aligned Stories Interpreted event as positive for feminism (n = 577) Feminism Attitudes 0.09 0.02 26.58 1 <.001 1.20 [1.06, 1.14]
Constant −1.31 0.22 34.32 1 <.001 0.27
Interpreted event as negative for feminism (n = 724) Feminism Attitudes −0.00 0.02 0.07 1 .787 1.00 [0.97, 1.03]
Constant 0.09 0.20 0.20 1 .655 1.09
Feminism-Misaligned Stories Interpreted event as negative for feminism (n = 998) Feminism Attitudes −0.03 0.01 5.51 1 .019 0.97 [0.94, 1.00]
Constant 0.50 0.17 9.09 1 .003 1.64
Interpreted event as positive for feminism (n = 276) Feminism Attitudes −0.03 0.03 1.35 1 .245 0.97 [0.91, 1.02]
Constant −0.19 0.33 0.33 1 .567 0.83
  • Note: The models for those who interpreted the stories as expected are shown separately to those who did not interpret the stories as expected. Significant predictors are shown in bold.

For the feminism-misaligned events, the model for those who interpreted the story as expected was statistically significant, χ2(1, N = 998) = 5.54, p = .019, R2 (Cox & Snell) = .01, R2 (Nagelkerke) = .01, and correctly classified 52% of cases. As shown in Table 3, there was a significant effect of feminism attitudes, such that for every one-point increase (i.e., more negative attitudes), the odds of participants identifying the feminism-misaligned story as fake were 3% lower. For those who did not interpret the story as expected, the model was not statistically significant, χ2(1, N = 276) = 1.38, p = .241, R2 (Cox & Snell) = .01, R2 (Nagelkerke) = .01.

Participants were again grouped according to their score on the feminism attitudes scale, for illustrative purposes. Figure 1 shows the rates of correct identification for each fabricated event across these groups. Note that only those who interpreted the event as expected (e.g., positive events as positive for feminism) are included here, those who did not interpret the event as expected were excluded.

Source: Online Library, Wiley

Share on facebook
Share on twitter
Share on whatsapp
Share on email

This website will be sold

Contact: author@sexandsexology.com

You may also like

subscribe to our newsletter

I expressly agree to receive the newsletter and know that i can easily unsubscribe at any time